目前中国能用的2bss

The US is experiencing another sharp increase in COVID19 cases. This is a simple fact, but as always seems to be the case in today’s world, this fact is being treated as an opinion. Countless people (including prominent politicians and even the president) are claiming that cases are not actually increasing, and the apparent increase is simply the result of increased testing. This claim is dangerous and untrue, but it also offers a good opportunity to teach some lessons in data analysis. Obviously, an increase in testing will result in an increase in the number of cases that are documented, that much is true, but that doesn’t necessarily mean that the entirety of the increase is from increased testing. So how can we tell whether the true number of cases is increasing? There are multiple ways to examine this, and I’m going to walk through several of them and try to explain the stats in a non-technical way so that everyone can really grasp these concepts.

To begin with, I’m not actually going to talk about coronavirus. That topic has, unfortunately, becomes such a political battleground (even though it should be entirely scientific) that it is difficult to get people to think clearly and unbiasdly about it. So instead, let’s start by talking about Willy Wonka’s chocolate factory. Like most chocolate factories, they sometimes get insects in their chocolate bars and they test subsets of them to see how often this occurs. This situation is analogous to testing for a disease, and the math is the same, so let’s use it as an example to understand the math, then we’ll apply that understanding to coronavirus.

For sake of example, let’s say that Wonka produces 10,000 chocolate bars a day, and examines 2,000 of them for the presence of insects (these are the tests). Further, as you might have guess, his chocolate factory has rather lax hygiene standards, so out of those 10,000 bars, 1,000 actually have insects. How many do we expect to have insects (i.e., be positive cases) in the sample of 2,000 tests? This is easy to calculate. 1,000 is 10% of 10,000, so we expect 10% of the tests to be positive. Thus, out of 2,000 tests, we expect to get 200 bars with insects (i.e., documented cases; note that I am acting as if testing is random to make the math easy for all to follow; this is a simplification, but doesn’t actually change the point; see note at the end).

【Green加速器电脑版下载2021】|Green加速器 PC端最新版「含模拟...:2021年8月28日 - Green加速器软件为印尼设计的网络速度计使电信用户能够准确地测量自己网络的速度。印尼各地都建立了专业的加速站,世界许多国家也建立了专业的加速站。下载Green加速器...

Figure 1: Changes in the percent of tests that are positive under different scenarios. For each line, testing increases by 10% of its starting value each day, but the number of actual cases (not observed cases) varies, and the lines show the percent of tests that were positive. Blue lines show a decrease in actual cases over time, the grey line shows no change in actual cases, and the red lines show an increase in actual cases. As you can see, anytime that the total number of cases increases, the percent of tests that are positive will increase, whereas if the total number of cases is unchanged or decreases, the percent of positives will either remain stable or decrease, even if testing increases.

The answer lies in the percentage of tests that are positive. If the actual number of bars with insects is unchanged, and the increase in positives is simply due to increased testing, then the percent of tests that are positive will remain constant even though the total number of positive tests goes up (Figure 1). Think about the math from earlier. 10% of bars have insects. So, we expect roughly 10% of tests to be positive, regardless of how many tests we do (though the percentage will be more accurate with a larger sample size). So, if we do 2,000 tests, we expect 200 bars with insects (10% positive). If we do 4,000 tests, we expect 400 bars with insects (10% positive). If we do 6,000 tests, we expect 600 bars with insects (10% positive), etc. The total number of bars with insects (cases) increase as testing increases, but the percentage of those tests that are positive remains the same. As another example, imagine that you have a bag with 500 blue marbles and 500 red marbles. You reach into the bag and grab a handful. You expect to get roughly 50% of each color regardless of how many you grab (though you expect the value to be closer to 50% [more accurate] as sample size increases). It’s the same with testing.

So, if the increase is entirely from testing, the percent of tests that are positive should be unchanged, but what happens if the number of insects in chocolate bars are actually decreasing, while testing is increasing? What happens then? Well, the total number of positive test results may either go up or down (depending on the sizes of the decrease in insects and increase in testing), but the percentage of tests that are positive will always go down (Figure 1). Going back to the example, we expect 10% of tests to be positive when 1000 out of 10,000 bars actually have insects and 2,000 tests are conducted. Now, suppose that the number of bars with insects is cut in half (500) and testing is tripled (6,000). Now, we expect only 5% of tests to be positive, but 5% of 6,000 is 300. So, while the total number of observed positive cases increased, the percent of tests that were positive decreased. This tells us that the actual number of bars with insects is decreasing, despite the increase in testing.

Conversely, if more bars actually have insects, we expect a higher percentage of tests to be positive, even if the level of testing increases. Imagine, for example, that the number of bars with insects increases to 2,000 out of 10,000, while the number of tests also doubles (4,000). Now, we expect 20% of tests to be positive, resulting in 800 cases. See how that works?

I have illustrated all of these patterns in Figure 1, showing the hypothetical situation I have been describing with changes in testing and, sometimes, changes in the actual number of bars with insects over a 20-day period. Each line shows the percent of tests that were positive. The grey line shows the situation where testing increases but the actual number of bars with insects (cases) do not, the blue lines show increased testing with a decrease in the actual number of cases, and red lines show increased testing coupled with an increase in the actual number of cases. As you can hopefully see, the only way to get a decreasing percentage of positive tests is if the actual number of cases (not simply the number of documented cases) decreases, and any time that the actual number of cases increases, the percent of tests that are positive will also increase. This percentage of positive tests is key for understanding what is actually happening.

Figure 2: Percent of coronavirus tests that were positive for June. The first panel shows the data for the whole country, and the second shows two states with large outbreaks (Florida and Arizona). They are presented in separate panels simply so that the change for the whole country is not obscured by the much larger change for individual states. Data were downloaded from the Covid Tracking Project late on 28-June-20.

Now, with all of that in mind, let’s look at coronavirus in the US. If the situation is truly improving and the actual number of cases is truly decreasing and the apparent recent increase in cases is just a result of increased testing, as many argue, then we should see that the percent of tests that are positive has continued to decrease. That is not, however, what we see. It was decreasing for a while, but if we look at June (when things have been opening back up and when the spike in cases occurred) we see a statistically significant (P < 0.0001) increase in the percentage of tests that are positive (Figure 2). In other words, the increase in tests simply cannot explain the entirety of the increase in cases. It probably is a contributing factor, but the actual the actual number of coronavirus cases in the US is actually going up rapidly. That is a fact. To be clear, exactly what is happening varies by states, and some cases are experiencing decreases in the rates of positive tests, but many others are experiencing sharp increases, particularly in states like Florida and Arizona (Figure 2). They are very much experiencing viral outbreaks (green加速器下载 has some very nice data and graphs for state data that I recommend looking at)..

green是这个吗?【游戏王决斗链接吧】_百度贴吧:2021年8月22日 - 加速器? 回复 来自Android客户端5楼2021-09-16 12:46 卡瓦伊可 新手1 1 怎么下载呀 还有别的VBN么 回复 来自Android客户端6楼2021-10-28 16:35 ...

So, what do we find for coronavirus in the US? Well, if we compare the last 7 days of May (7-day average) to the past 7 days of June (with the 28th being the most recent date based on when I downloaded the data), we find that the number of tests increase by 40.5%, while observed cases increased by 83.0%! In other words, the increase in cases substantially outpaces the increase in testing, clearly indicating that we are actually experiencing a real increase in coronavirus cases, not simply an increase in known cases due to increased testing. The situation is even more dire when you start looking at states where the largest outbreaks are occurring. In Arizona, for example, again comparing the last 7 days of May to the past 7 days of June, we find testing increased by 116.9%, but daily new cases increased by 498.2%. Florida is a similar story. Testing has increased by 88.3%, but daily new cases has increased by an astounding 726.7%! This is undeniably an outbreak.

Indeed, you can get a sense for these general trends just by looking at a comparison of testing rates and numbers of new cases over time (Figure 3). As you can see, at first, testing lagged well behind cases as we experience the first initial outbreak. Then, cases started declining, even though the number of tests continued a steady increase. It is only in the past few weeks (i.e., since social distancing restrictions, closures, etc. have been being lifted) that we see a spike in cases. Further, the recent spike in cases does not correspond to a spike in testing. Testing has been increasing at a steady rate, whereas cases suddenly shifted from a steady decrease to an exponential increase. In other words, the number of observed cases does not track well with the number of tests. If the current increase in cases was really a result of increased testing, then new cases should have been tracking with testing all along. They should have continued to increase after March, because testing increased. That’s not at all what we see, however. Again, testing simply can’t explain the trends. That doesn’t mean that there is no impact of testing, obviously there is, but it is clearly not the key thing driving trends.

Figure 3: Coronavirus testing and cases for the USA. As you can see, cases are a poor match for testing, indicating that testing alone does not explain the recent increase in cases. The x-axis labels show the start of each month. Data were downloaded from the Covid Tracking Project late on 28-June-20.

Yet more evidence comes from hospitalization rates. The “its just more testing” argument relies on the notion of many asymptomatic people (or at least people with very mild cases) that have only been detected recently due to increased testing. If that was the case, then hospitalization rates should be remaining level or going down (if the virus is truly going away), yet many states are experiencing increased hospitalization rates, with the Texas Medical Center (an enormous complex) hitting 100% capacity for its ICU. That simply cannot be explained as a result of increased testing.

Fortunately, deaths have not started spiking yet. There are several reasons for this. One is that, this time, more young people are getting the disease. Another is simply that death rates inevitably lag behind infection rates, and it is very likely that deaths rates will increase in the coming weeks (though many experts are hopeful that we will be able to avoid the type of enormous spike we saw a few months ago).

In short, an actual examination of the data clearly and unequivocally shows that the current increase in coronavirus cases in the US cannot be explained simply as a result of increased testing. The percent of tests that are positive is increasing, which is a clear indication that the actual number of cases is increasing. Further, in states like Arizona and Florida, the numbers are truly shocking, with the increases in new cases massively outpacing the increases in testing. We are clearly still in the middle of a deadly outbreak, and it is getting worse. This isn’t a liberal conspiracy to undermine Donald Trump; it is a fact, and facts don’t change based on your political party.

Note: Please refrain from political comments. This post is about science and evidence and comments should likewise be about science and evidence (see green加速器下载).

Note: someone might object that my examples assume random testing, while testing is actually somewhat targeted, and people who are symptomatic or are known to have been in contact with someone who is infected are more likely to be tested. This fact is true, but actually doesn’t substantially change anything I’ve said. It does affect the exact percentages but doesn’t change my point about the trends. It is still true that the only way to get an increasing percentage of positive tests while the testing rate is increasing is for the actual number of total cases to be increasing (technically, this could also happen if we learned to do a much better job at targeting our tests, but there is no indication of this that I have seen; certainly not enough to cause the numbers we are seeing, and it still would not explain the increases in hospitalization rates).

green加速器下载官网The data I presented here were downloaded from the Covid Tracking Project late on 28-June-20.

Posted in Uncategorized | Tagged Bad arguments, statistics | green加速器下载

目前中国能用的2bss

green加速器下载官网There are a lot of misconceptions about what science actually is, and, as a result, there are a lot of incorrect conclusions about the reliability and utility of science. I frequently encounter people who expect science to give absolute answers. They act as though science is a method for proving what is true with 100% certainty. As a result, they view cases where science led to an incorrect conclusion as evidence that science itself is flawed. You can clearly see this in arguments that a current scientific result doesn’t need to be accepted because “science has been wrong before” or “there used to be a scientific consensus that the earth was flat” (there wasn’t, but that’s another topic), etc. Similarly, there is a false view that a scientific conclusion is either 100% right or 100% wrong. In reality, science is a path to knowledge. It is a way of testing ideas and slowly building a body of knowledge based on the results of those tests. Sometimes, the path takes wrong turns, but unlike every other path to knowledge that has ever been invented, science is systematic and self-correcting and steers itself back in the correct direction, resulting in a gradual accumulation of knowledge.

Before I go any further, I want to acknowledge that this description of science as a “path to knowledge” is not original with me and was coined by my friend and fellow skeptic, The Credible Hulk. So, go check out their blog and Facebook page for more great science content.

I really love this description of science as a “path to knowledge” because it beautifully encapsulates what science is and why it works. You see, science does not give absolute results. In other words, it does not “prove” anything with utter certainty. Rather, science is all about probabilities. As I often like to say, science simply shows us what is most likely to be true given the current evidence. That probability can, however, always change with future evidence. Any scientific result can be overturned as new evidence comes to light.

The tentative nature of a scientific result is one of its great strengths, but it can lead to confusion. People often make the incorrect leap from, “science does not give definitive answers” to “science is uncertain and therefore I don’t have to accept a given result.” This is a flawed way of understanding science. Remember, it is a way for telling us green加速器下载官网 given the current evidence. Therefore, it’s results should be accepted until such time as future evidence arises to discredit those results. Sticking with our path analogy, a lack of 100% certainty that a path is going the right direction would not justify abandoning the path altogether and wandering aimlessly through the forest. Further, a lack of 100% certainty does not mean that we cannot be highly confident in a result. There are some things that have been so thoroughly tested so many times in so many ways that it is extraordinarily unlikely that they are wrong. In other words, some paths are marked well enough that you can be really confident in them.

On the other end of the spectrum, people ignore the tentative nature of scientific conclusions and act as though it should give definitive answers, leading to the flawed arguments about science having been wrong in the past. These arguments are problematic in a number of important ways. First, they treat the inherently self-correcting nature of science as if it is a bad thing, when in fact, it is another great strength of science. Really think about this. If you are going to argue that, “I don’t have to accept a scientific result because scientists used to think sun moved around the earth,” my question would be, “why do we no longer think that the sun moves around the earth?” The answer is very clearly that other scientists continued conducting tests and discredited the previous view. Science corrected itself. This is not a weakness, but rather a strength. No other path to knowledge does this. No other system of understanding repeatedly and systematically tests its conclusions and updates its information by rejecting debunked results and accepting new results.

Further, because of the way that science advances, the argument that “science has been wrong before” is inherently self-defeating. Sticking with the orbit of the earth for a minute, we only know that the earth orbits the sun because science debunked the notion that the sun orbits the earth, so you can’t use that as an argument that science doesn’t work, because the argument inherently includes the premise that science works! In other words, if this argument gives us carte blanche to disregard scientific results, then why should we accept the result that the earth moves around the sun? That result was produced by science, and this argument claims that we don’t have to accept scientific results, so why should we accept the result that the earth moves around the sun? We only know that science was wrong before because of science. Again, this self-correction is one of the best things about science.

Additionally, it is important to realize that scientific results are often incomplete more than actually wrong, and there are degrees of wrongness. The progression of physics is a great example of this that I use frequently. Newton made enormous strides in physics. He moved us far along the path, but we later found out that he was slightly off course. Einstein showed that Newton’s work was incomplete and his conclusions did not apply universally. However, that didn’t mean that we threw Newton out the window and went all the way back to the trail marker Newton started at. Newton moved us closer to the truth, and Newtonian physics are still taught and applied all around the world, but he was incomplete, and Einstein took Newton’s results and shifted us back on track. Think of it like this: we needed to go north, and Newton took us slightly north west. He still moved us much closer to our goal, but we needed Einstein to reorient us and get us back on track.

This gradual accumulation of knowledge is another key aspect of science. Yes, science sometimes makes mistakes, but because it corrects those mistakes, we gradually get closer and closer to the truth. People who thought the sun revolved around the earth were less wrong than people who though the sun was a god. Galileo was less wrong than the people who thought the sun moved around the earth. Newton was less wrong than Galileo. Einstein was less wrong than Newton, etc. At each step, we got closer, and closer to the truth. This is also another reason why it is so absurd to blindly disregard modern scientific results on the basis that science has been wrong before. Science is a gradual accumulation of knowledge, and although there certainly are things about which we are wrong today, we are less wrong than previous generations, and we know this because we tested the views of previous generations and built on that knowledge.

To give another example, there are certainly things about which modern medicine is wrong. That is inevitable due to the tentative and probabilistic nature of science, but modern medicine is less wrong than medicine was 20 years ago, and medicine 20 years ago was less wrong than medicine 40 years ago, and medicine 40 years ago was less wrong than medicine 60 years ago, etc. Further, I can demonstrate this extremely easily. Imagine you need a major medical intervention and you can be treated using the technology and knowledge from any of the following time points: 200 years ago, 100 years ago, 50 years ago, 25 years ago, or current. Rank your choice from lowest to highest. I’m willing to bet your choices went chronologically (inverse) with your preference being treatment via our current knowledge, and there is a very good reason why that is the correct way to rank things. Namely, science works! It’s not perfect, but it is a path that moves generally in the right direction, and we all intuitively realize that science has helped us progress and, thanks to science, we know more than any generation before us knew.

Further, we can extend my medical analogy to just about any field of science. Imagine that you are on a game show run by omnipotent aliens with a perfect knowledge of the universe. They ask you a chemistry question, and you have a lifeline that will let you call a random chemist from the current year, or from 25 years ago, or from 50 years ago, etc. Whom do you call? Obviously, you call the chemist from the current year. Again, we all intuitively accept that science works and gradually builds knowledge. Even those who like to argue that “science has been wrong before” must admit that, thanks to science, we know more now than at any other point in our history. Science has a proven track record of moving us in the right direction.

Finally, if you are not convinced by anything I’ve said thus far, then my question for you is simply, “what’s the alternative?” Really think about this. What other path to knowledge can compete with science? As I’ve explained before, science is responsible for our modern society. All of the technological and medical marvels around you are the result of gradually testing ideas and accumulating knowledge. Look at all the previously fatal diseases that we can now cure or even prevent, look at the decreases in mortality rates, etc. All of that is because of science. So why should we go back to unsystematic guess work? We tried other systems (like relying on anecdotes) for millennia, and they didn’t work. It was science that brought us out of the dark ages, and it is science that will allow us to continue our advancement as a species. Again, that doesn’t make science perfect or infallible. It simply shows us what is most likely true given the current evidence, but by constantly testing, by constantly self-correcting, by constantly updating, it gradually moves us closer and closer to the truth. It’s not perfect, and it certainly isn’t a straight path, but it’s the best path to knowledge that we have.

Note: To anyone who is about to reply with a snarky remark about doctors/scientists saying that smoking is safe, please read this post. The reality is that there was never a scientific consensus that smoking was safe and, in fact, science had showed that it caused cancer all the way back in the 1930’s. Indeed, actual studies consistently showed that it was dangerous. Tobacco companies simply did a good job of creating the illusion that science was on their side; meanwhile, actual science was continuing along the correct path.

Related posts

  • “But scientists have been wrong in the past…”
  • Most scientific studies are wrong, but that doesn’t mean what you think it means
  • No one thought that Galileo was crazy, and everyone in Columbus’s day knew that the earth was round
  • Settled science part 1: Is science ever actually settled?
  • Settled science part 2: Creating the illusion of a debate
  • Science matters because it works
  • Scientism: Is it a straw man or a legitimate critique?
  • Scientists aren’t stupid, and science deniers are arrogant

 

Posted in green加速器下载官网 | Tagged evaluating evidence, green加速器下载官网 | 29 Comments

目前中国能用的2bss

green加速器下载官网Asking questions is generally a good thing. Indeed, questions are the very foundation of science. People become scientists because they are curious and like to ask questions, and science itself is simply a systematic method for asking and answering questions. Unfortunately, the positive perception of questions often leads to people using questions as a disguise for wilful ignorance, and the phrase, “just asking questions” has been used to justify all manner of insane and illogical beliefs. The people who use this phrase are generally not actually asking questions. Rather, they are phrasing a belief as a question in an intellectually dishonest attempt to maintain the appearance on intelligence.

There are two major problems that I am going to discuss. The first is simply that not all questions are good. I fundamentally disagree with the notion that there is no such thing as a stupid question. Good questions stem naturally from known facts and evidence. In other words, they have a basis in reality. Bad questions, however, are not based on facts or evidence and instead rely on wild conjecture. Indeed, in science, hypotheses do not spring out of nowhere. Rather, they are based on the existing evidence.

Let me give an example. In my field (herpetology) there has been a fair amount of debate and discussion about the purpose of basking behavior in turtles (i.e., why do aquatic turtles come out of the water and bask on rocks and logs?). There have been many hypotheses/questions that people have looked at. For example, is it for thermoregulation (temperature)? Does it help immune functions? Does it remove parasites? Etc. All of these are good questions. They are perfectly rational things to wonder about based on our existing knowledge of biology.

Now, however, imagine a scientist asked, “Are they basking to avoid aliens that live in the water?” That would be a bad question, because it’s not based on any known facts. There is no reason to think that aliens are involved, and we’d need good evidence of the presence of aliens before it would be rational to even consider the possibility that they are involved. If a scientist asked that question at a conference, they would be laughed out of the room, and they absolutely could not justify it by saying, “I’m just asking questions. Aren’t you scientists supposed to be open-minded?” Yes, scientists should be open-minded, but being open-minded means being willing to accept new ideas when presented evidence for them. It does not mean being willing to accept or even consider the possibility of aliens influencing turtle behavior despite a lack of evidence that aliens are living in our aquatic ecosystems. Do you see the point? You can’t just say something insane that has no evidence to support it and justify it as, “just a question.” There needs to be some reasoning behind the question. There needs to be some actual evidence to make the question worth perusing in the first place.

网络加速器免费版_360百科:2021-12-19 · 1、体积小、加速快,是目前国内主流的网络加速器。2、无需注册,加速器自带test测试帐号密码,下载即可免费试用3天。3、全面覆盖国内主流网游,能有效解决您在网游中遇到的延时过高、登录困难、掉线频繁等问题,让网游从此畅爽无比。 4、优化教育网节点线路,解决教育网用户玩网游难的问 …

The second major problem with people “just asking questions” is that those questions are rarely good-faith questions being asked out of honest curiosity. Rather, they are often statements of belief that are being disguised as questions. Many (if not most) of the people asking things like, “did Bill Gates make coronavirus?” don’t actually want the answer. Rather, they are confident that they know the answer, and that’s a problem.

Asking questions is only a good idea if you are willing to accept the answers to those questions. In other words, asking a question like, “is coronavirus man-made” is fine if it is being asked out of a genuine sense of curiosity and desire for knowledge. There is nothing wrong with asking that question if you are then willing to look at the evidence and accept the answer provided by that evidence (in this case, the answer is a clear, “no, it was not man-made”). The problem is that many people asking the question won’t accept that answer. They refuse to accept the evidence, but also don’t want to admit that they are denying evidence. So, instead, they claim to be “just asking questions.”

To be clear, I don’t think most people are deliberately using the phrase “just asking questions” because they know that they are denying evidence and don’t want to look foolish. Rather, this is simply one of many cognitive traps that people fall into. Most of the people who go around justifying nonsense by saying that they are “just asking questions” probably truly think that they are being rational and are simply asking good questions. So, the point of this post is really to act as a warning. Be conscious of your views and biases, and if you find yourself “just asking questions” stop and ask yourself, “why am I asking this? Is there actual evidence to suggest that this is a good question?” Then, if you think that it is a good question, actually look at the evidence. If you aren’t willing to look at the evidence, then you are stating a belief, not a question. Once you’ve been shown the facts, it is no longer rational to keep asking the same question. Once you’ve been given the answer, your choices are either to accept it or deny it. You cannot claim to be rationally asking questions if you’ve already been given the answer to your questions and simply refuse to accept it.

Finally, it is worth explicitly stating that when I say to look at the evidence, I mean actual evidence from reputable sources. Youtube videos, conspiracy websites, outlets on either extreme of the political spectrum, someone you know on Facebook, a cherry-picked expert, etc. do not count. To quote Will Turner, “that’s not good enough.” In science, your evidence needs to come from the peer-reviewed literature, and you need to look at the entire body of literature, rather than cherry-picking, and for topics like politics and current events, you should get your information from multiple reputable news outlets. Don’t accept the first source you come across. Rather, cross-reference it using multiple other sources and see if they all say the same thing (the Media Bias Chart is a very useful tool for seeing if the sources you are using are neutral and reliable).

My point with all of this is simple. You should ask questions. You should think critically and evaluate what you are told, but your questions need to be based on known facts, and they need to be good-faith questions that are asked out of an honest curiosity. You must be willing to answer them by actually looking at evidence from reputable sources and accepting facts.

Related posts

  • Are scientists arrogant, close-minded, and dismissive?
  • Don’t mistake an assumption for a fact
  • Facts aren’t political (or religious)
  • Most scientific studies are wrong, but that doesn’t mean what you think it means
  • Research, you’re doing it wrong: A look at Tenpenny’s “Vaccine Research Library”
  • The Rules of Logic Part 6: Appealing to Authority vs. Deferring to Experts
  • What would it take to convince you that you were wrong?
  • green加速器下载官网
Posted in Rules of Logic | Tagged Bad arguments, evaluating evidence | 6 Comments

目前中国能用的2bss

By now, you have likely seen the viral video of two doctors in Bakersfield, California (Dan Erickson and Artin Massihi) holding their own press briefing in which they argued that COVID19 is no deadlier than the flu, shelter in place orders are doing more harm than good, and schools and businesses should re-open. Clips of the press briefing have rapidly been latched onto by many people for a variety of reasons ranging from political leanings to desperation for hope. Unfortunately, these doctors have no clue what they are talking about, badly blundered the statistics, made numerous false claims, and have enormous financial conflicts of interest (i.e., re-opening businesses would be tremendously financially beneficial for them). In short, they are emergency doctors, not virologists, microbiologists, immunologists, or epidemiologists, and they should leave the statistics to people who are properly trained to analyse them. To demonstrate that, I’m going to go through their nonsense point by point, starting with what I consider to be the core issues. Please note that I will only be discussing the science and logic behind sheltering in place, not the politics.

Note 1. I based this post on the entire 1+ hour interview, not the short 2–5 minute segments.

Note 2. It’s worth mentioning that the American College of Emergency Physicians condemned their statements.

Bad statistics

Incorrect mortality rates

Their entire argument rests on the notion that the mortality rate from COVID is actually very low, even less than 0.1% (roughly the typical mortality rate from the flu). Actual studies have found that the mortality rate varies from  3.6% (Baud et al. 2020) to 1.4% (Wu et al. 2020). I have yet to see an estimate based on confirmed cases that was anywhere near the number these emergency doctors came up with (see Note 3). So how did they get such a low number? Easy: they’re bad at statistics.

To get their numbers for a given county, state, or even country, they took a series of simple steps. First, they took the number of tests that had been conducted and calculated the percentage of positive results. Next, they “extrapolated” that by applying that percentage to the entire population of the geographic region in question to calculate the total number of positive cases. Finally, they divided the number of deaths by their calculated number of cases, and lo and behold, the death rates were low, way lower than actual epidemiologists have calculated (see example in Note 4). Why is that? Anytime you see one or two “experts” present a value that is vastly different from what all the other experts have arrived at, you should be suspicious, especially if they announce their findings in a blog, press conference, etc. rather than the peer-reviewed literature (where real scientists present their findings).

网游加速器-网游加速器大全-网游加速器专区-华军纯净下载:1970-1-1 · 网游加速器是能够让玩家远离卡顿的游戏,即使在玩国外的游戏时,也能体会到顺畅的网络。 酷跑网游加速器 版本: 2.9.20.417 官方版 大小: 11.83MB 语言: 简体中文 内容简介: 酷跑网游加速器是一款针对网游开发的,高稳定性的游戏加速器。

Coronavirus testing thus far has been a very biased sample. It has not been truly random sampling. Rather, it has been heavily biased towards people who had symptoms, people who were in contact with someone who developed COVID19, people at high risk, etc. In other words, the percentage of positive cases in the testing is probably much higher than the actual state or country-wide percentages, just as our estimate of dark blue marbles was unrealistically high. This means that our intrepid doctors overestimated the total number of cases, thus vastly underestimating the mortality rate. They calculated mortality by dividing known deaths by their estimated cases, which means the higher the number of estimated cases, the lower the death rate.

The other problem is that they are only using the people that have died thus far, but that number is going to keep going up, even if no one else ever becomes infected. In other words, some of the people who are infected with COVID right now are going to die. So, you can’t take the ongoing infection data and divide the number of deaths by the number of cases, because people are still dying. That number is going to keep going up. To illustrate, let’s say that we have 10,000 currently infected people, plus another 10,000 who have either died (100) or recovered (9900). It would be stupid to take those deaths (100) and divide by all those cases (20,000) and conclude that there is only a 0.5% death rate. We can’t do that because we still have 10,000 people who are infected, some of which will die. Do you see the point? Using these numbers midway (as they did) biases the results towards a lower death rate.

These very basic problems with their analyses completely nullify their results. The numbers they are basing their arguments on are invalid, which means that they have nothing to back up their claims.

Note 3: Both the number of confirmed cases and the number of confirmed deaths are almost certainly large underestimates, but since we don’t know those values, it’s hard to know what the true death rate is. That does not, however justify the type of shoddy statistics they used. Also, there have been several recent prevalence estimates based on antibody tests that argued for a much higher disease prevalence than is currently documented, but the estimates thus far have been riddled with problems (including non-random sampling) that are beyond the topic of this post.

Note 4: To work through the math of one of the examples they gave, at the time they collected their numbers, California had done (according to them) 280,000 tests, with 33,865 positives, giving a prevalence of roughly 12%. They then assumed that 12% applied to the entire state and multiplied 0.12 by 39.5 million (CA’s population), resulting in 4.7 million calculated cases (according to them; they didn’t round correctly). Now, if we just divide the current number of deaths (1,227) by that number and multiply by 100, we get a mortality rate of 0.03%. (I have not checked their numbers as far as number of cases and such; I’m just reporting their shoddy math).

Sweden vs Norway

A core piece of these doctors’ argument is that sheltering in place doesn’t work. They claim that there is peer-reviewed evidence to support this (I haven’t seen any), but the only “evidence” they present is a comparison between Norway (which shut things down) and Sweden (which did not shut things down officially, but still many work from home, engage in social distancing, etc.). The numbers, according to them (which seem at least close to correct for a few days ago) are as follows: Norway (shut down) has a population of 5.4 million and has 7,191 confirmed cases and 182 deaths. Sweden (not shut down) has a population of 10.4 million and has 15,322 cases and 1,765 deaths. According to them, those death rates are “statistically insignificant,” a term that they clearly don’t understand. You can’t just eyeball the numbers and assert that they aren’t significant. You have to actually do some statistical tests. Based on those numbers, Norway has had 34 deaths per 1 million people. In contrast, Sweden is all the way up at 170 deaths per million! If we do an actual statistical test (chi square), that difference is, in fact, highly significant (P < 0.0001; this means that there is less than a 0.01% chance that a difference this great or greater could arise by chance; more on stats here). Again, these guys are emergency doctors, not statisticians or epidemiologists. They are talking about things they do not understand.

Now, to be clear, that comparison I just made is not great either. I only made it to show the absurdity of their claim that those numbers aren’t different. The reality is that there are tons of differences between those countries that make it very difficult to make such a comparison. For example, not only did Norway lock down, it also has done substantially more testing than Sweden which can also have a huge effect (it’s also why the number of cases per million is similar between the countries even though the per capita mortality rate is so different). Any sort of country comparison like that is inherently problematic, particularly if you have a sample size of 2 countries. Also, a better approach is to look at trends, not snapshots. For that, I’d take a look at the graphs presented by the BBC (they are on a log scale, so the difference between Norway and Sweden is substantial). My point is simply that their analysis is totally bogus. Once again, the data aren’t on their side.

Shutting things down works

Another core piece of their argument is that shutting things down and sheltering in place aren’t effective. This is based on, as far as I can tell, utterly nothing. They claim there are studies to support their claim, but they don’t cite them and I can’t find them. They also try to use the Norway/Sweden comparison, but as I showed above, if anything that actually suggests that shutting down works. Finally, when pressed by a reporter for their evidence, one of them seemed frustrated and said, “I don’t need a double-blind clinically controlled trial to tell me if sheltering in place is appropriate. That is a college level understanding of microbiology.”

Now that statement is interesting for a number of reasons. First, up until that point (it was late in the interview), they kept insisting over and over again that they were just following the science. They repeatedly claimed to be the ones objectively looking at facts. Yet when pressed for their evidence, they retorted by saying they didn’t need studies, because they just knew (a very common science-denier strategy). Further, the effectiveness of sheltering in place is clearly not something you’d test with a double-blind clinically controlled study, which makes me suspect that they know very little about experimental design.

apex加速_apex高速下载_apex免费加速_腾讯网游加速器:腾讯网游加速器限免费加速APEX英雄,支持加速origin平台加速,APEX游戏下载加速,apex游戏加速。 腾讯公司 | 版权所有 Copyright ...

Death rates are low because we took action. Also, the outbreak is ongoing

At several points, they criticized the early models that predicted hundreds of thousands or even millions of deaths. They cited them as evidence that people over-reacted and the disease is not much deadlier than the flu. This argument is, however, based on a poor understanding of the models. These sorts of models don’t show what will happen, rather they showed what could happen under a range of scenarios. We run them precisely so that we can change our behaviour and avoid the worst outcomes. That’s literally their purpose. We don’t run them for the fun of it. We run them so that we can learn how to save lives. The number of deaths is much lower than originally predicted green加速器下载官网we shut down schools, implemented shelter in place orders, etc. The things that these doctors want to undo are the very things that prevented us from having millions of deaths. This is very much like anti-vaccers arguing that we don’t need the measles vaccine because measles deaths are rare. They are only rare because most of the population is vaccinated. Even so, the death rates are “low” because we implement measures to make them low.

The other thing to keep in mind is that the situation is very much ongoing. At the time I’m writing this, the US has nearly 57,000 deaths from COVID-19, and it is still adding well over 1,000 (often over 2,000) deaths daily. Erickson frequently cited annual US deaths from the flu as being 24,000-62,000 (at another point they said 37,000–67,000; it’s actually 12,000­–61,000). He used this as evidence that COVID is no worse than the flu, but stop and think about that for a second, we are already at nearly the highest end of that range, and given the rate at which people are dying, we are going to shoot well past it, and that is green加速器下载官网. Just think how much worse this would be if things weren’t shut down! The only reason they are even able to make that comparison is because we shut things down. They are simply wrong that COVID isn’t deadlier than the flu.

Comorbidity doesn’t mean COVID isn’t responsible

They also spent a great deal of time arguing that COVID isn’t green加速器下载the killer, it’s actually the other conditions like being immunocompromised, being a smoker, etc. This is a very stupid argument. Yes, most mortalities are associated with other factors, but that does not in any way, shape, or form change that fact that those people would not have died if they had not caught COVID. Indeed, that’s a big part of why keeping things shut down is so important: it protects the people who are the most vulnerable.

Interestingly, this argument is another one that they lifted straight out of the anti-vaccine playbook. Anti-vaccers frequently make the same argument claiming, for example, that measles doesn’t kill anyone; it’s the secondary infections that kill. This, of course, ignores the fact that those infections happen because of the measles. Even so, yes, COVID generally has help in killing patients, but that doesn’t negate its role. This argument is like talking about a gunshot victim and arguing that, “Bullets aren’t dangerous, because the bullet didn’t kill him; it was green加速器下载官网 the loss of blood.” It’s a very dumb argument.

 Sheltering in place won’t destroy your immune system

A final core thrust of their argument is the notion that sheltering in place will harm your immune system and make you sick. You see, according to them, viruses and bacteria are the “building blocks” of your immune system, and if you aren’t regularly exposed to them, they will all disappear somehow, they won’t protect you, and your immune system will be weakened. They also extended this to arguing that you shouldn’t disinfect things in your house, shouldn’t wear a mask in public, etc. They justified this by smugly saying, “we’re not wearing masks. Why is that? Because we understand microbiology, we understand immunology, and we want strong immune systems.”

To quote a famous meme, “That’s not how this works, that’s not how any of this works!”

There are a ton of issues here, but let me start by acknowledging the grain of truth in their silo of stupidity. It is true that you have a microbiome consisting of many bacteria, viruses, etc. and they do play important roles in your body, potentially including helping fight some diseases. Also, there is some evidence that exposure to microbes early in life helps to train the immune system to respond correctly, and a lack of those microbes results in autoimmune disorders and allergies (this is known as the “hygiene  hypothesis”).

None of that, however, supports their claims. First, there is no reason to think that staying home for a month or two is going to dramatically alter your microbiome. They acted as if all the bacteria living in and on you will die if you don’t go outside. That’s nuts. They will keep living and reproducing and doing bacterial things. You are their home. That’s where they live. To be clear, a change in your routine might shift the microbiome around slightly, but it is constantly shifting around slightly, and there is no scientific evidence that sheltering in place is going to shift your core microbiome in a detrimental direction, and it certainly isn’t going to deplete your body of bacteria.

Further, they acted as if your house is totally sterile (except when it was convenient for them to act otherwise; see later), which is insane. Even if you disinfect your counter (as they waxed on about), your house is crawling with bacteria. Do you live with other people? They have bacteria. Do you have a pet? They are coated in bacteria. Even inside, you are constantly exposed to bacteria other than the ones on and in you.

Additionally, microbes are not the “building blocks of the immune system.” They aren’t even part of the immune system. Sure, they train your immune system, but only in that it learns which microbes to attack (and how to attack them) and which ones not to attack. Many people (these guys included) talk about exposure to bacteria “strengthening your immune system” as if exposure to bacteria A will result in a general improvement in your immune system and ability to fight other bacteria, but that simply isn’t how it works. As I explained in detail here, exposure to bacteria A simply teaches your immune system how to kill bacteria A and whether it needs to. It doesn’t “strengthen” it against other bacteria/viruses.

Note 5: Unlike these two, I have actually published papers on host microbiomes.

They lied when they said experts agreed with them

At one point, they said that they had shown their results to local health officials, and those officials agreed with them and were just waiting on permission from the governor to re-open things. That was, however, a lie. A spokesperson for the Kern health department said “our director has not concurred with the statements that were made yesterday about the need to re-open at this time.” As a general rule, I don’t trust people who make such brazen lies.

They aren’t experts/don’t cherry-pick your doctors

This is a point that I have touched on repeatedly throughout, but it is worth stating again: these two are emergency care doctors, not microbiologists, not immunologists, not virologists, not epidemiologists. They are not experts on a topic like COVID. They are not people you should be treating as authorities. Having an MD does not make you an expert on all aspects related to medicine. They state early on that they have taken courses on these topics, which I’m sure they did back in pre-med/med school, but that doesn’t make them experts. I took courses on these topics, as well, as part of my training in biology, and, as part of my PhD, I even studied microbiomes and the effects of an emerging infectious disease on the ecology of wildlife populations, but that doesn’t make me an epidemiologist. The fact that I have a little bit of training and experience in that field does not make me an expert in it, and it certainly doesn’t put me on par with people like Fauci who have spent their entire lives studying these topics.

When pressed on why actual infectious disease experts fundamentally disagree with them, they first tried to dodge the question by going on rabbit trails about how Fauci’s actions weeks ago were justified because he didn’t have all the data (which completely ignores the actual question of why people like Fauci still disagree with them now). Then, they eventually argued that the disagreement was because people like Fauci have just been doing research from afar for years, whereas they are “in the weeds” seeing how things are on the ground. This is, of course, an insane argument. Beyond the fact that many (probably most) other healthcare workers who are “in the weeds” with them green加速器下载 (we’ve all seen the photos of nurses blocking protesters who are trying to open things up), treating COVID patients does not in any way shape or form make someone an expert on the factors and conditions that allow the virus to spread. Emergency patient care and epidemiology are two very different things and doing one does not qualify someone as an expert in the other.

Nevertheless, I’m sure there will be some who continue to insist that these two know what they are talking about because they are doctors, at which point my question becomes, “why trust them?” There are thousands of doctors with far more relevant experience who disagree, so why trust these two? Why cherry-pick them out of all the experts? What makes them more trustworthy than all the other MDs and PhDs? Is it possible that you are blindly believing them not because they have good data (they don’t) or because they are experts (they aren’t) but rather simply because they are saying the things you want to hear? You should carefully consider this possibility, because it is a very easy cognitive trap to fall into.

Massive conflicts of interest and probably biases

green加速器官网下载地址 最新官方APP怎么下载_green加速器..._九游:2021年6月12日 - green加速器怎么下载?想要比别人更加抢先抢快的玩到这款游戏,那么你获取游戏开测消息是关键,能够获取到第一手信息,你才能在最快的时间内容体验到,...

513vpn网络加速器9.1破解版_软件下载_3G免费网:2021-7-10 · 3G免费网www.3gmfw.cn为你分享513vpn网络加速器9.1破解版,513vpn 513vpn网络加速器 513vpn网络加速器破解版的相关软件下载:513vpn网络加速器9.1破解版去除非功能类的多余信息 Diy了界面 让界面看着更简介舒适!去除了登录与官方非VIP用户 ...

There also seem to be some very strong political biases at play. Erickson hinted at a conspiracy throughout, with frequent statements about “something else going on.” For example, he asserted (with no evidence whatsoever) that ER doctors were being pressured to write “COVID” on death certificates for some political motive. “Why are we being pressured to add COVID? To maybe increase the numbers and make it look a little bit worse than it is? I think so.” This is a nonsense conspiracy that I have not been able to find any evidence to support. Further, he admitted that it would be administrators who would be pressuring doctors, but when asked why administrators (who are hurting financially from COVID) would try to artificially increase the death rate, he didn’t have an answer.

Things really went off the rails after the main chunk of the interview (and a related moment part way through) where Erickson started ranting about the word “safe.” In regards to official health recommendations, he said, “when they use the word safe, the word safe, if you listen to the word safe, that’s about controlling you.” Ah, the ubiquitous and amorphous “they.” Who is “they”? Who knows? Probably some evil government entity. Later he said (his emphasis), “who says what’s safe? Are you smart enough to know what’s safe for you or is the government gonna tell you what’s safe for you…They are using this to see how much of your freedom can they take from you.” This is, of course, pure pablum. I wonder if he also applies this line of reasoning when the government tells people how to be “safe” when a hurricane is coming. I also wonder if he has more trust for health officials when they determine the “safe” doses of drugs. It’s just insane to argue that health guidelines designed for public safety are really about public control, and, more importantly, it makes his biases abundantly clear. It is extremely obvious that he is motivated, at least partially, by a strong distrust of the government. Despite all of the claims to be just looking at the evidence, they gave a lot of indications of following political biases, not facts.

Note 6: In cases like this, the general public does not know enough to know what is safe. To be clear, it’s about knowledge, not intelligence, but most people simply don’t have the training and experience to actually evaluate epidemiological data and determine what is safe. That’s why we should listen to experts like Fauci, not some random person on the street. This assault on expertise is yet another very common anti-science strategy.

Miscellaneous

At this point, I have covered the bulk of this nonsense and hit the most important points, but I want to touch on a few more minor issues.

Quarantining the healthy

At several points they asserted that quarantines have never applied to healthy people before. This is a lie. During the 1918 Spanish flu outbreak, things were shut down very much as they are today.

Coronavirus on plastic objects

At one point, they argued that because coronavirus can live on plastic for up to three days, sheltering in place is pointless because you are just bringing it into your house when you buy things. They even asserted that they can probably find COVID in your house. This is just stupid. The odds that a shovel from Home Depot (to use their example) has COVID on it are very low, and much, much lower than the odds that one of the many people you’d be near in Home Depot would give you COVID (seriously though, you probably don’t need to go to Home Depot right now). The fewer people you are in contact with, the lower your odds. It’s that simple. So yes, sheltering in place absolutely does minimize your risk.

Also, note that by claiming that things you bring into your home commonly harbor micro-organisms, they have just totally negated their own argument about staying at home preventing you from being exposed to bacteria and viruses. You can’t have it both ways.

The need for more testing

At one point, they accidentally gave up the game and said, “In order to re-open the economy, you have to have widespread testing, that’s #1.” That is actually something I agree with. If we had sufficiently wide-scale testing, we could start slowly re-opening because we could monitor cases and quarantine the sick. The problem is that we aren’t there. Not even close. We don’t have nearly enough testing to be able to do that. Thus, this admission defeats their entire argument.

When a reporter asked when they thought testing would be sufficient, they dodged the question and started arguing that people aren’t getting tested because they are just so scared to leave their home (never mind all the testing shortages).

More contagious than the flu

They also shot themselves in the foot by admitting that COVID19 is more contagious than the flu. Think about this with me. Their core argument is that COVID’s death rate (i.e., deaths among infected individuals) is the same as the flu’s. Even if that were true (it’s not), COVID would green加速器下载be more dangerous because of the higher transmission rate. Being more contagious would mean more people get it, and, as a result, more people die.

Claimed New York ordered 30,000 ventilators and used 5

视频加速器永久免费版|迅雷视频加速器下载官方 ...- 系统天堂:2021-8-8 · 本站提供视频加速器永久免费版下载,迅雷视频加速器免费版是专门为广大的网友推出可以对各种在线网络视频播放加速,不仅可以有效的增进缓存,还能节省宽带资源的占用,其采用先进的网络加速技术,为广大用户提供一个完全免费的视频加速平台。

green网络加速器-greenvp 1.2.4 安卓版-新云软件园:2021-6-29 · 新云小编带来的green加速器APP,无需注册! 提供多种优质线路,体验超快、流畅! 一键连接,立即使用,高速稳定不掉线!感兴趣的朋友快来新云下载吧!

Green网络加速器gjsq.me - 网站排行榜:Green网络加速器是一款绿色、稳定、好用的加速软件, 同时支持windows、iphone、ipad、android和Mac

Conclusion

In short, these doctors have no clue what they are talking about and seem to be motivated by money and politics, not science. Their statistics are bogus and their facts are faulty. ABC should be ashamed of itself for broadcasting their nonsense.

I think information should be shared and available. So you may re-blog, re-post, quote, etc. as you see fit, just please include a link/credit back to my page. Thanks

Posted in Uncategorized | Tagged green加速器下载, Bad arguments, cherry picking | 1 Comment

目前中国能用的2bss

Are there more than two sexes? This is a question that has caused an enormous amount of social and political debate in recent years, but at its core, it is a scientific one, and I want to treat it as such. In other words, what we do with the answer to that question certainly has social and political ramifications, but the question itself is one of biology, not politics. Therefore, I am going to try to answer it in this post from a strictly scientific standpoint. I am not going to make any statements about politics, morality, religion, etc. Instead, I am going to talk only about the biology. As always, if you are going to read this, then all that I ask is that you lay aside any ideologies and views you might hold and look solely at the facts. Political and social positions must be based on facts, not the other way around. So, in this post, all I am going to do is present the facts.

Terms, definitions, and critical background information

On topics like this, it is always a good idea to define the opposing positions at the outset. In this case, there are basically two camps. One holds that sex is strictly binary and is determined by the presence or absence of a Y chromosome (sometimes stated more explicitly as XX = female, XY = male). The other position argues that sex is more complicated than this binary and follows more of a spectrum rather than a clear dichotomy. Some people misunderstand this and construct a straw man about people arguing for the existence of “third sex.” The argument is not that there is a third sex, but rather that sex cannot be adequately defined by two discrete categories because there are many people with both male and female traits. In other words, one position argues that sexes can be defined as two distinct boxes into which all individuals fit. The other argues that the situation is more complex and there are some individuals who do not fit cleanly into either box and are actually somewhere in the middle. I think part of the confusion arises over the way that we talk about this, and I fully admit that I have been guilty of this as well. We often say things like, “there are more than two sexes” as convenient shorthand, but what we really mean is that sex cannot be adequately defined using a simplistic dichotomy in which all individuals with a Y chromosome are males and all individuals without a Y chromosome are females. It is more complicated than that, and there are many intersex individuals that do not fit neatly into traditional categories of males and females.

Note: I opened with the question, “Are the more than two sexes?” simply because that is terminology that is familiar to most readers and introduces the topic.

Note for clarity: based on the comments thus far, I want to clarify that I don’t have a problem with a more nuanced position that says something to the effect of, “based on reproductive physiology most individuals are binary in that they either have the physiology for producing sperm or the physiology for producing eggs, but there are a variety of exceptions to this. So the binary classification is useful in some contexts, but we should acknowledge that there are exceptions and situations where the binary classification does not work.” Indeed, that is more or less what I am arguing (also note that I am only talking about biological sex, not gender or gender identity).

Next, we need to talk about how we define sexes, and before we get specifically to humans, it is really important to look at the biology of sex more broadly, because this gives us context and important background information. So, let’s start with the general definition of male and female. If you were to ask professional biologists to provide a general definition of “male” and “female,” the one answer you are not going to get is, “if a Y is present it’s a male; if Y is absent, it’s a female.” There’s a very good reason biologists don’t use that definition. Namely, because it doesn’t work for a very large number of organisms. You see, many organisms don’t have sex chromosomes; instead, male vs female is determined by some environmental factor (I’ll come back to that in a minute). Further, even for species with sex chromosomes there are lots of exceptions and atypical situations (again, more later).

Because of these problems biologists have historically defined sex based on the production of gametes (sperm and egg). The sex that produces small (usually mobile) gametes is considered to be the male, and the sex that produces the large, stationary gametes is considered to be the female. Thus, it is the production of gametes that defines sex, not the presence of a particular chromosome. To put that another way, sex is defined by gamete production, but in some cases, it is determined by chromosomes. In others, it is determined by environmental factors. This may seem like pointless semantics, but it is actually really important (as will become increasingly clear as we go), because the biological definition of sex is not about chromosomes. This already puts the “Y = male” position on shaky ground (it’s also worth noting that in many species it is the female that has two different sex chromosomes, not the male).

Having said all of that, there is a caveat that needs to be explained. Namely, the broad definition of male vs female that I have given can run into trouble at the individual level because some individuals are sterile, so by this definition, it seems like they simply shouldn’t have a sex. In reality, we define sex practically based on the physiology that would result in the production of a particular gamete under normal circumstances. This is important, because physiology is rarely binary. There aren’t, for example, two distinct groups of people with regards to metabolism: high and low. Rather, there is a whole spectrum of metabolic activity.

The next thing we need to talk about is genotype vs phenotype. The genotype is what a person is genetically. In other words, what their genes code for, whereas the phenotype is the physical characteristics of the individual. This is important to understand because different genotypes can lead to different phenotypes, but also the phenotype does not always match the genotype. This becomes particularly true when we start talking about epigenetics. An epigenetic effect occurs when something other than genetics affects the expression of the trait. In other words, the phenotype is determined not only by the genotype, but also by the environment, enzymes, etc. and in some cases, those factors can override the genotype.

Sexes in the animal kingdom

With all of that background in place, let’s look at the animal kingdom and see what sort of variation exists for the sexes, because there is a lot we can learn from this broad perspective (I promise I will talk about humans later). Even a cursory knowledge of zoology will quickly tell you that sex is complicated. There are, for example, many species that are hermaphrodites. This means that they simultaneously have the physiology to produce eggs and the physiology to produce sperm. They are not “male” or “female;” they are both.

Many other organisms can switch between the sexes, and in many cases do so obligately (i.e., all individuals start out as one sex and switch later in life). This is one of the places where epigenetics comes in. Anemonefish (aka clown fish) are a good example (Todd et al. 2016). Anemones are inhabited by a male-female pair, where the female is larger and dominant. Individuals start off life as males and pair up with a female, but if that female dies, this causes epigenetic changes in the male, resulting in it changing sexes and becoming a female. Thus, if Finding Nemo was biologically accurate, when Nemo’s mother died, Marlin (his father) should have changed sex and become Marla.

In many other species, individuals do not change sex as adults, but their sex is determined by the environment as they develop. Some (but not all) turtles provide a good example of this (as do crocodilians, some lizards, etc.). They are what we call temperature sex determined (TSD), and the temperature at which the eggs are incubated determines the sex of the offspring. I don’t want to get too technical here (and indeed there are important pieces of information that we don’t have yet), but I do want to briefly walk through some of how this works because it is instructive (see a more detailed overview here: Lance 2009). During early embryonic development, sex has not been determined (this is true in humans as well) and whether an embryo becomes a male or a female depends on the hormones present. Under many conditions, the embryo will develop as a female, and this seems to be largely driven by the hormone estradiol, which is made from testosterone via the enzyme aromatase. At certain temperatures, however, aromatase stops converting testosterone into estradiol, ultimately resulting in the development of male characteristics.

I went through all of that info on TSD because that background knowledge lets us look at some import questions. For example, what happens if we raise eggs at a male-producing temperature, but we supply them with estradiol? The answer is usually that females develop (green加速器下载). In other words, even though temperature usually determines sex we can over-ride that and produce a different sex. Further, the fun doesn’t stop there, because in at least some cases, we can take turtle species that do have sex chromosomes, paint the eggs with estradiol, and get hatchlings with female physiology even if they are genetically male (Freedberg et al. 2006)! In other words, we can make turtles that have male sex chromosomes develop female phenotypes, including the ability to lay fertile eggs. This is why I’ve been arguing that chromosomes sometimes determine sex, but they don’t define it. We can change the sex to be something other than what was determined genetically. To put that another way, even though chromosomes usually determine sex in these species, we can override that and make the estradiol treatment determine sex.

Similarly, there are some lizards that are usually genetically sex determined (i.e., sex is based on chromosomes) but at certain temperatures, there is an epigenetic effect and the temperature overrides the genetics and determines the sex of the hatchlings. In bearded dragons, for example, at high temperatures, animals that are genetically male (based on chromosomes) develop as females and produce fertile offspring (green加速器下载官网). So, if you want to insist that chromosomes define sex, rather than determining it (under normal circumstances), then you must claim that these lizards who are running around laying fertile eggs are actually males. This is a notion that any biologist would scoff at because, again, that’s not how we define sex. If you are going to claim that males are laying eggs, then you have invented your own definition of “male” that biologists do not accept.

Finally, you may be wondering, given all this complexity with TSD and chromosomes, can you ever get intermediates? The answer is, yes! There are situations where individuals don’t develop entirely as male or entirely as female and instead end up developing partially as both (Ewert and Nelson 1991), which makes it pretty impossible to maintain a view that sex is binary. In other words, up until this point, you could have tried to make a post hoc change to the original argument and claim that, “there are only two sexes, and it is determined by physiology,” but that doesn’t work, because some individuals have aspects of both male and female physiology.

The point that I’m trying to get at here is that sex is complicated. It is clearly not as simple as a binary state determined strictly by chromosomes, because we know that you can have reproductive “females” who are genetically “males.” We know that there is more to sex than simply the chromosomes. and we know that environmental factors can override the genetics. Now, you may protest to this because I have been using examples from non-human animals, but that counterargument misses the point. The point is that traits are more complicated than a simplistic understanding of genetics would lead you to believe, and there is no reason to think that sex is only complicated in non-human animals. Indeed, as I’ll explain in the rest of the post, sex is extremely complicated in humans. To put that another way, using non-human animals is a good way to get people to lower their biases and look at the evidence, and as you’ll see, the bizarre situations in other animals are highly analogous to what happens in humans.

green加速器下载官网

Let’s being by looking just at the sex chromosomes. In humans, you have probably heard that there are two possibilities for sex chromosomes: XX and XY, but that is not correct. In reality, there are many possible combinations, and it’s not that uncommon for someone to have an atypical number or arrangement of sex chromosomes. Indeed, one large study found that 1 out of every 426 people (2.34 out of 1,000) had one of these conditions (Nielsen and Wohlert 1991).

For example, some people get extra X chromosomes. When this is associated with a Y chromosome, it is known as Klinefelter syndrome, and people with it can be XXY, XXXY, or even XXXXY. These unusual genotypes are associated with a combination of male and female phenotypes (with female traits being more prominent when more X chromosomes are present). People with this condition have male genitalia, but they are often have small testes and are sterile or have reduced sperm counts, they have less body hair and often no facial hair, they have lower testosterone levels, and in some cases they develop breasts (green加速器下载). So here, we have people who have two X chromosomes, but also a Y chromosome, breasts but also a penis, testes but low testosterone levels, etc. They simply don’t fit neatly into the discrete boxes of “male” and “female.”

Extra X chromosomes can also occur without the presence of a Y, and you can have someone who is XXX (sometimes called “superfemale”). People with this present mostly as normal female phenotypes, but they are taller on average, and often have learning disabilities (Tartaglia et al. 2010). Things often become more severe when there are four X chromosomes (“tetrasomy X”; XXXX). Some people with this develop normally, but others do not experience normal puberty, don’t develop a normal female phenotype, and are infertile. Beyond this, some individuals actually have a full 5 X chromosomes (XXXXX) and experience even more severe symptoms. Here again, we have atypical chromosome arrangements resulting in different phenotypes.

There can also be unusual numbers of Y chromosomes. For example, some people are XYY. These individuals have mostly normal male phenotypes and are usually fertile. Others may have XYYY or even XYYYY. These conditions are quite rare making it hard to generalize, but behavioral problems such as aggression have been reported in several cases (Abedi et al. 2018).

Additionally, there are XXYY individuals. These individuals are largely similar to XXY individuals, though there are some differences (green加速器下载官网). Like XXY individuals, they are generally sterile, and have reduced male features (e.g., small testes).

Finally, there is a condition known as an X monosomy (Turner syndrome; XO). This occurs when an individual has a single X chromosome and either no Y or sometimes a partial Y. These individuals appear female, but are generally infertile and do not have properly developed gonads (Fryns and Lukusa 2005). I want to pause here for a second to note that you can get a situation where someone has part of a Y chromosome. So if your definition of sex is based on the presence or absence of a Y, how do you define someone who has part of a Y? Are they only partially male?

By this point, it should be abundantly clear that sex in humans is far more complicated than XX vs XY, and there are lots of genotypes and lots of phenotypes. It should be obvious that chromosomes determine sex rather than defining it, but there are still more layers of complexity that we haven’t gotten to yet. What if I told you, for example, that it is possible to be born with normal female genitalia, even though you have a Y chromosome? This is a condition known as Swyer syndrome, and it’s often a result of a mutation on the SRY region (aka testis-determining factor) of the Y chromosome, but many other genes can cause it as well (Thomas and Conway 2014). These genes often play key roles in activating the right chemicals for an embryo to develop into a male (think back to the turtles earlier for an analogous situation), so when they are modified, those chemicals don’t get produced at the right amounts. As a result, people with Swyer syndrome have a predominantly female phenotype, but instead of having either testicles or ovaries, they have “streak gonads” which are undifferentiated pieces of tissue that can produce neither eggs nor sperm. People with this condition typically don’t go through puberty and require hormone treatments to develop secondary sexual characteristics such as breasts. However, people with this condition can usually carry a child and give birth if an embryo is artificially implanted. I want you to stop and think for a second about just how complex this is. Here we have people who have a Y chromosome, but also have vaginas, don’t have either testes or ovaries, but have all the other female reproductive physiology and can carry a child if implanted with it. The line between male and female is really blurred in this situation.

The inverse of Sewyer syndrome is “XX male syndrome.” This condition produces individuals with typical male genitalia despite the fact that they do not have a Y chromosome. The cause of this is usually a mutation that resulted in the SRY region ending up on an X chromosome (Anik et al. 2013). Much like Sewyer syndrome, individuals with this condition are generally sterile and often have reduced testes.

There are other situations that are even more bizarre. For example, there are documented cases of people developing “ovotestes.” These are gonads that have some of the features of a testis and some of the features of an ovary. This often occurs in people who are XX but have a mutation on the RSPO1 gene (Tomaselli et al. 2011), which results in ambiguous gonad development. Others actually have both an ovary and a testis and were historically referred to as “true hermaphrodites.” This can occur in both XX and XY individuals (though XX is more common) as well as individuals with some of the chromosome abnormalities described earlier. Further, some individuals with this condition are actually fertile and have children (this usual happens when one gonad is developed and the other is an ovotestis; Krob et al 1994). In other words, there are people who are reproducing even though they have both ovarian and testicular tissue (this is more common in mothers but there also people who are fathers despite this condition). You may remember from the beginning of this post that biologists have typically defined sex based on the physiology required for producing sperm vs eggs. So how are we supposed to classify these individuals who have both physiologies?

There are also cases of individuals who are chimeras. In other words, they have two sets of DNA, and in some cases, one of those sets is XX and the other is XY. In some cases, this has little effect on individuals, and they can reproduce, but in other cases, it results in the development of either ovotestes or other odd combinations of gonads as described earlier. Nevertheless, some of these individuals can still reproduce (Verp et al. 1992). To put that another way, there are people who have a Y chromosome, and have testicular tissue, but still produce eggs and give birth. Now, if you are going to insist that things are as simple as, “if you have a Y you are a male,” then you must argue that these people are males, even though they have mostly female phenotypes and give birth. This is, again, not something that any of the biologists I know would accept.

Beyond all of that, we know that there are epigenetic effects at play in sexual development (Gunes et al. 2016). There are, for example, epigenetic effects on the expression of the SRY region. Exactly how this plays out in developmental sex disorders (DSD) is still poorly understood because epigenetics is such a new field, but we know that there are epigenetic effects that influence the development and expression of male and female traits (phenotypes), and as this field expands, it is likely that we are going to discover that sex is even more complicated than we currently realize (we’ll have to wait and see).

green加速器下载

As you can hopefully now see, the topic of sex is extremely complicated, and there is far more to it than simply XY = male, XX = female. There is a whole suite of genotypes and phenotypes, including individuals that are XO, XXX, XXXX, XXXXX, XXY, XXXY, XXXXY, XYY, XYYY, XYYYY, and XXYY. Further, there are individuals who are XX yet develop mostly as males, and there are individuals who are XY but develop mostly as females. There are literally people who give birth, despite having a Y chromosome. There are people who have both ovaries and testicles. There are people who only have part of a Y chromosome, etc.

So, if you are going to insist that Y = male, you are going to have to make some bizarre claims. For example, you are going to have to say that XY individuals with an SRY mutation are, in fact, males, despite the fact that they were born with vaginas, lack testicles, and, if implanted with a fertilized egg, can carry a fetus to term. You are literally going to have to say that a male can give birth. Similarly, you are going to have to say that some XX individuals are females, despite the fact that they have mostly male physiology (including penises). Those are, of course, nonsense positions that biologists don’t accept. Biologically, sex is defined by the physiology needed to produce particular gametes (eggs or sperm), not by sex chromosomes, but recent years have shown that this simply is not a binary situation. There are many individuals that have aspects of both male and female physiology, thus making it impossible to use binary categories.

Let me put that another way. Given the existence of individuals with conditions like XXY who have some female traits and some male traits, the existence of individuals who appear female despite being XY, the existence of individuals with both an ovary and a testis, the existence of people who give birth despite having a Y chromosome, etc., which of the following descriptions seems more accurate, “sex is strictly binary; if you have a =Y you are a male, if you don’t you are a female, no exceptions” or “sex is a complex trait with many genotypes and phenotypes as well as epigenetic factors. It is a spectrum of traits and cannot adequately be described using strictly binary categories.” Which of those does a better job of describing the enormous variation that I have discussed in this post?

Again, to be clear, I’m not making any political or social arguments here. What you do with this information and how it affects your views is up to you, but you must accept facts, and the facts clearly show that biologically, sex is more complicated than a simple binary dichotomy.

Rules for commenting on this post

 As explained, this post is solely about the science. If you think I am wrong about the science, feel free to explain, but I do not want the comments to divulge into endless political and social debates. As I said, for the sake of this post, I am just presenting the science. What you do with that is up to you. Comments that are not about biology or that tack political arguments onto biological ones will be deleted. Similarly, if you think I am wrong, please actually explain why rather than just saying, “no, Y = male.” Actually deal with the points I raised and evidence I presented. Also, be civil (see the Comment Rules for my more general policies).

Literature cited

(see this post if you have trouble accessing these for free)

  • Abedi et al. 2018. Rare 48, XYYY syndrome: case report and review of the literature. Clinical Case Reports 6:179–184.
  • Anik et al. 2013. 46,XX Male Disorder of Sexual Development: A Case Report. Journal of Clinical Research and Pediatric Endrocrinology 5:258–260.
  • Ewert and Nelson 1991. Sex determination in turtles: diverse patterns and some possible expliantions. Copeia 1991: 50–69.
  • Freedberg et al. 2006. Long-term sex reversal by oestradiol in amniotes with heteromorphic sex chromosomes. Biology Letters 2
  • Fryns and Lukusa 2005. Monosomies. Encyclopedia of Life Sciences.
  • Gunes et al. 2016. Genetic and epigenetic effects in sex determination. Birth Defects Research Part C Embryo Today Reviews 108:321–336
  • Holleley et al. 2015. Sex reversal triggers the rapid transition from genetic to temperature-dependent sex. Nature 523: 79–82.
  • green加速器下载. True hermaphroditism: Geographical distribution, clinical findings, chromosomes and gonadal histology. European Journal of Pediatrics 153:2–10
  •  Lance 2009. Is regulation of aromatase expression in reptiles the key to understanding temperature-dependent sex determination? Journal of Experimental Zoology 311:314–322.
  • Nielsen and Wohlert 1991. Chromosome abnormalities found among 34,910 newborn children: results from a 13-year incidence study in Arhus, Denmark. Human Genetics 87:81–83.
  • Tartaglia et al. 2008. A new look at XXYY syndrome: Medical and psychological features. American Journal of Medical Genetics A. 146A:1509–1522
  • green加速器下载. A review of trisomy X (47, XXX). Orphanet Journal of Rare Diseases 5
  • green加速器下载. Swyer syndrome. Current Opinion in Endocrinology & Diabetes and Obesity 21:504–510.
  • Todd et al. 2016. Bending genders: The biology of natural sex change in fish. Sexual Development 10.
  • green加速器下载. Human RSPO1/R-spondin1 Is Expressed during Early Ovary Development and Augments β-Catenin Signaling. PLoS One 6:e16366
  • Verp et al. 1992. Chimerism as the etiology of a 46,XX/46,XY fertile true hermaphrodite. Fertility and Sterility 57:346–349
  • Visootsak and Graham 2006. Klinefelter syndrome and other sex chromosomal aneuploidies. Orphanet Journal of Rare Diseases 1.
green加速器下载官网 Uncategorized | Tagged Bad arguments | 29 Comments